Competition among human women has probably contributed to hidden ovulation

Competition for pairs between prehistoric human women could have contributed to “hidden ovulation” – the lack of any notable physical evidence that a woman is fertile, experts say.

Using computational models, American researchers have found evidence that hidden ovulation in humans – which is unusual in the animal kingdom – has evolved to allow women to hide their fertility from other females.

This would have helped to avoid female conflicts, probably caused by aggression towards potential rivals for male colleagues.

Previously, scientists believed that women evolved to hide ovulation from men to encourage them to help care for children.

New research shows that the origin of hidden ovulation could actually have been more female-oriented than previously thought.

Human females have evolved to hide the physical signs of ovulation - which means men are no wiser

Human females have evolved to hide the physical signs of ovulation – which means men are no wiser

“The study of human evolution tended to look at things from a male perspective,” said the study’s lead author, Athena Aktipis, an associate professor of psychology at Arizona State University in the United States.

Even women’s specific adaptations – such as their social behavior and hidden ovulation – have been viewed in terms of how men shape them.

“Our calculation model shows that female sociality is much more than securing male investment.”

It is considered that human females have hidden ovulation, because there is no external physiological sign, either for the woman herself or for other people, that ovulation occurs.

As a result, women rely on useful aids, such as charts, test strips, applications or wearable technology to identify periods of fertility.

In contrast, some animals, such as baboons, undergo obvious physical changes during a period of ovulation – in particular, swelling of the perineal skin.

Gradually, during human evolution, female fertility is likely to become increasingly difficult to detect from the observer’s point of view.

For almost half a century, the evolution of hidden ovulation in human women has been explained by a theory called the male investment hypothesis.

Human females rely on aids such as diagrams, test strip applications, smart monitors and wearable technology to identify fertility periods.

Human females rely on aids such as diagrams, test strip applications, smart monitors and wearable technology to identify fertility periods.

In essence, the theory suggests that hidden ovulation has been helpful in securing male partners to help raise and support children.

This hypothesis has been the predominant explanation for hidden ovulation for decades, although it has undergone few empirical tests and has not been formally modeled so far.

But female primates do not only interact with males – they interact with each other, sometimes cooperating and sometimes involving conflicts.

“I’ve been puzzled by the male investment hypothesis for years, and because you can’t argue with a verbal hypothesis, I’ve started working on how to test it,” Aktipis said.

At the same time, Aktipis was working on “female sociality” – a term used to describe female individuals in a population of animals that tend to associate in groups.

“It struck me that women could have assaulted other females who showed ovulatory signs, which would create a benefit to hide ovulation.”

Sexual swelling in a baboon.  In general, the skin surrounding the perineum of a female baboon shows cyclic changes in size, color, and firmness during a menstrual cycle.

Sexual swelling in a baboon. In general, the skin surrounding the perineum of a female baboon shows cyclic changes in size, color, and firmness during a menstrual cycle.

This theory, called the “female rivalry hypothesis,” is now an alternative and convincing argument for how hidden ovulation has evolved.

Ovulatory cues would have made women more obvious as potential loving rivals for a man.

Evolutionary adaptations in humans take place on the time scale of many generations, which makes it difficult to test whether or how traits might evolve.

Therefore, Aktipis and colleagues tested the female rivalry hypothesis using computer modeling, which allows researchers to test ideas that would be difficult to test in the real world.

In agent-based computational models, an “agent” is a person whose behavior can be programmed and analyzed.

Each agent follows a specific set of rules and can interact with other agents and the environment.

In the model developed to test the female rivalry hypothesis, male and female agents followed the rules governing their movement, reproductive behavior, and attractiveness.

Male agents varied in their promiscuity – promiscuous males did not associate with females to help raise offspring, while non-promiscuous male agents blocked out to share resources and to support future children.

Female agents had either physical clues that indicated when they were ovulating, or ovulation was hidden.

Female agencies could also behave aggressively towards each other.

Female and male agents interacted with each other and had opportunities to procreate and form parental partnerships.

The model supported the hypothesis of female rivalry, showing that women who hid ovulation did better, the team found.

They had several children, avoided female-female aggression and managed to form parental relationships with men.

“Working in the social sciences has tended to assume that male knowledge and behavior is implicit,” said study author Jaimie Arona Krems, an assistant professor of psychology at Oklahoma State University.

“But women repeatedly face some unique challenges, especially in their interactions with other women.

“This work is the result, in part, of taking the idea seriously.

“When we do that, I think we’ll learn more, not just about the female mind, but about the human mind.”

The research team also used the model to test the male investment hypothesis, creating scenarios that did not allow women to behave aggressively with each other.

But there was no clear benefit from hiding ovulation in this scenario, again suggesting that hidden ovulation evolved due to interactions with other females.

“This work is a necessary change in thinking about how human females have evolved,” said Aktipis.

“Women’s sociality and other adaptations are not just about ensuring male investment, even though this has long been the underlying assumption about the purpose of female social behavior.”

The study was published in Nature Human Behavior.

How do women realize when other females ovulate using clues on their faces – and how can they then try to hide their partners from the “threat” of these fertile women?

Experiments in 2016 showed that women can also observe females ovulating simply by looking at their faces.

And it is believed that this “ability” could help women keep their partners.

Recent research has shown that men find images of ovulating women more attractive than the same people, taken in the later, less fertile phase of menstrual cycles.

In fact, women have been reported to dance, walk, play, smell and look more attractive on the most fertile days.

The ability of men to recognize the signs of fertility is useful to them, because it increases their chances of transmitting their genes.

While some experts believe that there is no benefit for women who can take each other’s ovulatory cues, others say it is useful when it comes to competition for men.

Researchers at the University of Bern, Switzerland, have created experiments to explore how fertile and non-fertile women interact.

They hypothesized that ovulating women may be perceived as a reproductive threat and draw men away from their partners.

In an online study, 160 women received pairs of images in which an image was generated when one woman was the most fertile and another in the least fertile phase of the cycle.

Participants were asked to indicate which face they found more attractive.

Another experiment in a laboratory performed the same test with 60 women.

In addition to choosing the most attractive face, these participants were asked which woman would be more likely to steal their own date.

Blood tests were performed to record women’s hormone levels.

Both experiments showed that, unlike men, women did not find “fertile” or “less fertile” faces more attractive to each other.

This came as a surprise to researchers who expected women to find the faces of their fertile peers more attractive.

But women with “natural cycling”, with higher levels of estradiol, who were not on the pill, were more likely to choose a fertile face as a woman to attract them.

“These results imply a role for estradiol [a type of oestrogen produced in the ovaries] when evaluating other women competing for reproduction, “the study, published in the Royal Society’s Journal, says Biology Letters.

The finding is consistent with studies that find positive associations between estradiol levels and competitive behavior among women, such as a greater emotional reaction to sexual infidelity.

“Our data suggest that in women estrogen is more related to intra-sexual competition than testosterone,” the scientists write.

“Ovulatory women (who are currently fertile) pose a greater threat to women with high levels of estradiol (who are not currently fertile but have a high fertility potential).”

.Source